Logo-jrhs
J Res Health Sci. 21(4):e00529. doi: 10.34172/jrhs.2021.66

Review Article

Effects of Light on Attention and Reaction Time: A Systematic Review

Rostam Golmohammadi 1, Hanieh Yousefi 2, Negar Safarpour Khotbesara 1, Abbas Nasrolahi 3, Nematullah Kurd 1, *
1Department of Occupational Health and Safety Engineering, School of Public Health, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
2Department of Ergonomics, School of Public Health, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
3Research Center for Prevention of Psychosocial Injuries, Ilam University of medical science, Ilam, Iran
*Correspondence: Nematullah Kurd (MSc) Tel: +98 918 3354890 E-mail: kurd_ohse@yahoo.com

Abstract

Background: Accuracy, speed, efficiency, and applicability of activities in the workplace are among the most important effective factors on people's productivity, which is in turn affected by environmental factors, such as light. Therefore, the present research aimed to review the studies performed about the effects of light on attention and reaction time.

Sudy Design: A systematic review.

Methods: This review study systematically searched articles from 2000-2019 in databases of Google Scholar, ISC, SID, Magiran, Web of Science, Science Direct, PubMed, and Scopus using keywords of light, lighting, attention, and reaction time. The titles and abstracts of articles containing relevant results over the past 20 years were extracted. Thereafter, they were categorized and analyzed according to the title, author name, publication year, study method, study type, and evaluation results.

Results: Based on the results, the light with shorter wavelengths, higher intensity, and higher color temperature led to suppressed melatonin, higher consciousness, less somnolence, increased attention, and faster reaction time. Simultaneous exposure to harmful levels of environmental factors affects cognitive and physiological parameters, acting independently with a separate mechanism or synergistically with a similar mechanism. The best light in the regulation of psychological, biological, and cognitive processes is bright daylight in the morning with a short wavelength, high intensity, and more lasting effects.

Conclusion: As evidenced by the obtained results, light is a powerful modulator of non-visual performance in cognitive tasks. The wavelength, color temperature, and light intensity modulate brain responses to cognitive tasks, including attention and reaction time. Therefore, these parameters, along with personal and environmental factors, should be considered in designing and using light.

Keywords: Light, Lighting, Attention, Refractory Period

Copyright

© 2021 The Author(s); Published by Hamadan University of Medical Sciences.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.


Introduction

Today, modern technologies have changed the working environment, creating more visual and cognitive needs than just physicalones 1 . Based on the studies conducted in recent decades, good illumination conditions modulate human needs regarding working, economic, environmental, and design-architectural requirements. Human performance, apparent space, safety, health, and well-being are improved by taking advantage of good lighting conditions 2 . Accuracy, speed, efficiency, and applicability of activities in the workplace are among the most effective factors on people's productivity, which is in turn affected by environmental factors, such as light 3 . The human visual system does not work optimally in poor lighting which leads to information loss, increased errors, and decreased performance 4 .

Numerous studies have demonstrated that proper lighting exerts a positive impact on work performance, reducing accident rates. Moreover, inadequate lighting increases eye strain, reduces performance, and leads to accidents 4-6 . Human factor research on lighting has largely focused on light visual aspects, as well as visual disturbance and performance. Evidence on the non-visual, psychological, and biological effects of light has recently been presented 7 .They revealed that different lighting conditions significantly affect many non-visual functions, such as physiological and psychological mechanisms, and biological-cognitive processes, such as Circadian Rhythm, consciousness, core body temperature, hormone secretion, and sleep 8-10 . Furthermore, several laboratory studies have pointed out that exposure to higher levels of illumination leads to lower melatonin secretion, increased physiological arousal, higher consciousness, as well as improved continuous attention and cognitive function 11,12 .

Attention and reaction time are among the important human cognitive indices. Attention is a cognitive process defined as a selective focus on one aspect of the environment while ignoring others 13 . The word "attention" can be defined in accordance with the number of errors made during a test. Accordingly, more careful attention during the test leads to fewer errors and vice versa. Furthermore, there is a close relationship between attention and reaction time 14 . That is to say, the higher levels of attention result in a shorter reaction time, and the opposite is also true. Reaction time is the time elapsed between understanding a situation and the response provided by an individual 15 . In humans, it may last from 0.5-> 3 sec, depending on the type of activity, attention, and consciousness 16,17 .

Some studies assessed the effect of lighting on cognitive functions; nonetheless, they have not reached a clear and definite conclusion 18. According to the aforementioned issues, although various studies have been conducted on cognitive functions and their importance, there is a dearth of research pertaining to the effect of inappropriate lighting on cognitive functions, including attention and reaction time. Attention and reaction time have a significant role to play in human errors and the occurrence of accidents; therefore, it is highly important to analyze the influential factors affecting them in the workplace.


Methods

This review study investigated the effects of lighting on attention and reaction time. To collect the required data, a query was conducted on six available electronic databases, including Google Scholar, ISC, SID, Magiran, Web of Science, Science Direct, PubMed, and Scopus. The search was performed using the keywords of light, lighting, attention, and reaction times. At each stage, the searched articles in each database were entered into the endnote software. In the first stage, a total of 187 documents related to the topic were entered into the software. In the second stage, according to the framework selected for the study based on a review of published studies from 2000-2019, the relevant documents before this period were deleted, yielding 101 articles. Since many of the records found in various databases were indexed, duplicates were inevitable. Therefore, in the third stage, the duplicates were removed; as a result, 90 documents remained for review. In the next step, the titles of these articles were carefully reviewed, and 21 irrelevant ones were deleted. After the revision of their abstract, another 30 documents were excluded from the assessment due to their irrelevant methodology. Following that, the complete file of the remaining articles was received; however, the full text of three articles could not be accessed, and they were excluded from the review process. The examination of the full texts revealed that 11 articles were not closely related to the subject in terms of purpose, method, and results; therefore, they were ruled out. A diagram of the study selection process is displayed in .

jrhs-21-e00529-g001
Figure 1. Flowchart of the literature review

For performing the study, one of the researchers was assigned to review the literature and check for the inclusion and exclusion criteria based on the title and abstract. After the removal of the articles that failed to pass the inclusion criteria, the full text of all the selected ones was investigated. Thereafter, the desired results were extracted considering a number of focused parameters and handed over to another researcher to review and revise them, if necessary. In general, out of 101 documents, 19 articles were investigated to extract the results.

In this study, independent variables included light characteristics (e.g., intensity, wavelength, and color temperature), environmental factor (e.g., noise, heat, and vibration), and personal factors (e.g., gender, sensitivity, and duration exposure), while attention and reaction time were regarded as dependent variables. Each of the reviewed articles examined several variables; therefore, the results of the reviewed articles were classified and evaluated in three main areas: (1) the effects of light-related factors on attention and reaction time (Table 1); (2) the combined effects of light and other environmental factors on attention and reaction time (Table 2); and (3) the effects of personal factors related to light sensitivity on attention and reaction time (Table 3). Moreover, the effects of daylight and other comprehensive studies on cognitive processes, including attention and reaction time, were investigated.

Table 1. Articles related to the effects of light factors on attention and reaction time
Line Title Authors Study method Study type Results
1 Effects of bright and blue light on acoustic reaction time and maximum handgrip strength in male athletes: a randomized controlled trial 29 Knaier et al.
2017
A number of 74 male athletes were randomly allocated to bright light (BRIGHT), monochromatic blue light (BLUE), or a control condition (CONTROL). Light exposure lasted for 60 min and started 17 h after the individual midpoint of sleep.ExperimentalBright light might reduce melatonin levels; nonetheless, neither bright nor blue light exposure in the evening seems to improve reaction time or handgrip strength in athletes.
2 A higher illuminance induces alertness even during office hours: Findings on subjective measures, task performance, and heart rate measures 11 Smolders et al.
2012
The study employed a mixed-group design (n=32), testing effects of two illuminance levels (200 lx or 1000 lx at eye level, 4000 K) during one hour of morning versus afternoon exposureExperimentalEffects of illuminance on subjective alertness and vitality, sustained attention in tasks, heart rate, and heart rate variability
3 Effects of blue light on cognitive performance 30 Bansal et al.
2017
During the 5-hour daytime study, seven healthy male participants were exposed to two different screen interfaces for three and half hours (3:30) under a controlled environment.ExperimentalContinuous exposure to LED screens brought about a decrease in frontal region delta theta activity and increased alertness.
4 Effectiveness of classroom lighting colors in students’ attention and meditation extracted from brainwaves 37 Bozkurt et al.
2014
Attention and meditation levels are extracted from the observed brainwaves of randomly selected two students when changing classroom lighting colors.ExperimentalThe attention level of subjects was higher in the red lightning color of the classroom, in comparison with white and green colors.
5 Bright light effects on working memory, sustained attention, and concentration of elderly night shift workers 33 Kretschmer et al.
2012
A group of four subjects took various visual cognitive performance tests simultaneously in a closed 5.2m-7.4m-3.8m room with a rectangular base area.ExperimentalBright light at night reduced error rates for a working memory task and a concentration performance task.
6 Effects of correlated color temperature on focused and sustained attention under white LED Desk lighting 40 Huang et al.
2014
Three correlated color temperature conditions (2700, 4300, and 6500 K) were examined, and the Chu Attention Test was used to measure focused and sustained attention.ExperimentalCorrelated color temperatures affected attention. In specific, the 4300 K condition resulted in significantly better focused and sustained attention.
7 Evaluation of the effects of different levels of
lighting on attention and reaction time under
laboratory conditions 59
Dehghan et al.
2017
A number of 33 subjects (age range of 19-26 years) underwent cognitive tests. Participants were exposed to three levels of lighting (200, 500, and 1500lux) in laboratory conditions while performing continuous performance tests.ExperimentalSignificant differences were observed in the percentage of attention and reaction time in different levels of lighting.
8 The relationship of sleep quality and mental fatigue in different levels of lighting on attention and reaction time in thermal comfort condition 67 Mohebian et al.
2016
The participants were exposed to 500, 200, and 1500 lx lightings for 1.5 h at a temperature of 22Cº to take the continuous performance test, the reaction time test, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory (PSQI), and the mental fatigue checklist.ExperimentalThe relationship of sleep quality and mental fatigue before and after exposure to different levels of lighting with the reaction time was significant.
9Luminance contrast has little influence on the spread of object-based attention68 Watson et al.
2013
The interaction between selective attention and luminance contrast with a contour-grouping task that provides a sensitive measure of the spread of object-based attention was investigated.ExperimentalThe spread of object-based attention was largely independent of contrast.
10 The effect of blue-enriched white light on cognitive performances and sleepiness of night-shift workers: A field study 26 Motamed adeh et al.
2017
Participants were exposed to two lighting conditions for a week. Subjects performed the Conners' Continuous Performance Test II (CPT-II) and 1- back test. A melatonin assessment was carried out.Before-after interventionalCompared to normal lighting conditions, omission errors and the reaction time during the sustained attention task decreased.
11 Effects of blue and red-enriched light on attention and sleep in typically developing adolescents 28 Studer et al.
2018
The adolescents participated in two lab days, which took place one week apart. A lab day consisted of two sessions in the light lab, one in the morning, and one in the evening, each lasting for about 1.5 h.Cross-overBeneficial medium to large effects of blue-enriched light on attentional performance in two out of three was tasks observed.
Table 2. Articles related to combined effects of light and environmental factors on attention and reaction time
Line Title Authors Study method Study type Results
1 Effects of combined exposure to noise, heat, and lighting on cognitive performance 42 Amiri et al.
2015
A number of 128 subjects within the age range of
24-18 years were selected from among the students of Shiraz University of Medical Sciences. The Tool to evaluate cognitive function in this study was Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test.
ExperimentalThe mean score of attention and working memory were reduced with worsening combined conditions.
2 Investigating the combined effects of heat and lighting on students’ reaction time in laboratory conditions 20 Mohebian et al.
2016
The study was conducted on 33 healthy students in a thermal stress chamber. The reaction time was measured by a reaction time measurement device after exposure to different heat surfaces and lighting surfaces.ExperimentalReaction times and reaction time error increased after combined exposure to heat and lighting.
3 Effects of noise, heat, and indoor lighting on cognitive performance and self-reported affect 46 Hygge et al.
2001
A factorial between-subject design was employed with three independent variables: Noise (38 and 58 dBA), Heat (21 and 278C), and Illuminance (300 and 1500 lx).ExperimentalInteractions were found between noise and heat on the long-term recall of a text and between noise and light on the free recall of emotionally toned words.
4 Combined effect of whole-body vibration and ambient lighting on human discomfort, heart rate, and reaction time 47 Monazzam et al.
2018
Participants were subjected to 12 experimental steps, each lasting 5 min for four different vibration accelerations in X, Y, and Z axes at a fixed frequency. Three different lighting intensities of 50, 500, and 1000 lx were also considered.ExperimentalThe combined effect of vibration and lighting had no significant effect on any of the discomfort, heart rate, and reaction time variables.
5 Evaluation of the Combined Effects of Heat and Lighting on the Level of Attention and Reaction Time: Climate Chamber Experiments in Iran 69 Mohebian et al.
2018
Study was conducted on 33 healthy students (17 M/16 F). The attention and reaction time tests were performed by continuous performance test and the RT meter, respectively, in different exposure conditions.Empirical An increase in heat and lighting level caused a decrease in average attention percentage
and correct responses, as well as an increase in commission error, omission error, and response time.
Table 3. Articles related to effects of personal factors related to light sensitivity on attention and reaction time.
Line Title Authors Study method Study type Results
1 Gender differences in light sensitivity impact on brightness perception, vigilant attention, and sleep in humans 55 Chellappa et al.
2017
Potential gender differences to evening light exposure of 40 lx at 6500 K (blue-enriched) or at 2500 K (non-blue-enriched), and their impact on brightness perception, vigilant attention, and sleep physiology were investigated.ExperimentalIn contrast to women, men showed a stronger response to blue-enriched light in the late evening even at very low light levels.
2 Circadian and gender differences after acute high-altitude exposure: Are early acclimation responses improved by blue light?74 Silva et al.
2015
A number of 57 volunteers were randomly assigned to two groups: nocturnal (2200-0230 h) or diurnal (0900-1330 h) and exposed to acute hypoxia (4000 m simulated altitude) in a hypobaric chamber.ExperimentalSome tendencies toward better cognitive performance (d2 attention test) were observed under blue illumination.
3 Light effects on behavioral Performance Depend on the Individual State of Vigilance 64 Correa et al.
2016
Hypothesis was tested by measuring the participants' behavioral state of vigilance before light exposure using the Psychomotor Vigilance Task.ExperimentalParticipants with higher levels of basal vigilance before light exposure benefited most from blue-enriched lighting, responded faster in the Sustained Attention to Response Task.

Results

The selected articles were published in Persian (n=5) and English (n=10) journals. They were generally experimental studies and encompassed various aspects of light and its effects on cognitive functions, including reaction time and attention. The list of final articles which met all the study criteria is presented in Tables 1, 2,3.


Discussion

In the process of human-machine perception, cognitive activities, such as reaction time and attention, are considered structural elements and main cognitive reactions to external stimuli in order to understand and analyze the conditions of assigned task 19,20 . Human factor research on lighting has largely on light visual aspects, as well as visual disturbance and performance. Evidence on the light non-visual, psychological, and biological effects has recently been presented 7 . According to various studies reviewed in the present research, the effects of lighting on attention and reaction time can be analyzed as the following:

For indoor lighting, illuminance is one of the important factors which can indicate the quality of lighting conditions. In their study, Yang et al 21 indicated that illuminance significantly affected subjects’ attention and alertness (P<0.05). That is to say, higher illuminance leads to higher levels of alertness and attentiveness. The participants were most alert, least relaxed, and performed most concentrated under lighting conditions of 500lx. Another laboratory study illustrated that even in the absence of sleep and light deprivation, exposure to a higher illuminance at the eye level can induce subjective alertness and vitality, increase physiological arousal, and improve performance on a sustained attention task 22 . The same results were reported by Leichfried et al who concluded that early morning illumination improves subjective alertness and mood; nonetheless, it had no impact on melatonin level and mental performance of individuals 23 . Generally speaking, a high illuminance level could make subjects feel more alert and concentrated, pointing to its significance in the enhancement of people’s attention level.

Numerous studies have pointed to the positive effects of blue light on function and consciousness 24,25 . For instance, Motamedzadeh et al 26 demonstrated that compared to baseline conditions and 6500 K, blue-enriched white light (17000 K) effectively improved working memory and sustained attention of control room staff. The performance results of the participants in tasks requiring sustained attention in the mentioned study have also indicated that exposure to blue-enriched white light does not affect the error of action. Nevertheless, compared to baseline conditions, such exposure significantly reduced the number of deletions and response errors. Moreover, the results observed a significant difference between the mean removal error at 17000 K in blue-enriched white light and baseline conditions (P=0.020).

In a laboratory study, Baek and Min 27 showed that exposure to blue-enriched white light after lunch reduced alpha-band activity and improved sustained attention. In the same context, Studer et al. 28 reported that participants demonstrated increased attention in two of the three attention-based tasks due to blue-enriched light, compared to red-enriched light in the morning (high illuminance about 1000 Lux and short duration less than 1 h). The results also indicated a reduction in reaction time in the performed tests. Along the same lines, Knaier et al 29 have found that differences in reaction time in the control group (compared to other participants) were 1 and 2 milliseconds (95% CI-9.5) for participants in white light and blue light conditions, respectively.

Bansal et al 30 also detected significant differences in screened factors, EEG delta/theta activity, mood, sustained attention (reaction time tasks), short-term memory (verbal memory task), and working memory (visual memory task) due to exposure to blue-enriched white light. The effects of blue-enriched white light on the improvement of cognitive function have also been reported in other studies, including Cajochen et al. 31 , Viola et al 24 , Vetter et al 32 , Kretschmer et al 33 , and Chellappa et al. 34 . Nonetheless, previous studies have proved that blue-enriched white light may lead to retinal damage and oxidative stress. Therefore, in order to reduce the negative effects of blue-enriched white light, its direct exposure is prevented 35,36 . Bozkurt et al 37 , in their study on the level of attention in two students with the lighting color change in the classroom, have found that the attention level in both subjects was higher in red light, as compared to that in white and green lights.

Some studies have addressed the effects of correlated color temperature (CCT) on visual function or its physiological and psychological effects 38 . Chellappa et al 39 have observed that fluorescent lamps with higher color temperatures could enhance consciousness, well-being, and visual comfort. Moreover, exposure to a color temperature of 2700 K led to a faster reaction time in tasks requiring sustained attention, as compared to a color temperature of 6500 K. Their results further have revealed that higher color temperature requires proportional attention in tasks due to its more melatonin suppression and faster reaction time.

Huang et al 40 have pointed to the effects of color temperature on focused and sustained attention under white LED desk lighting. In the stated study, three CCTs conditions (2700, 4300, and 6500 K) were evaluated, and the Chu attention test was used to measure focused and stable attention. A paired comparison of CCT conditions suggested that at 4300 K color temperature, the score was higher than other conditions. In other words, the focused and stable attention is higher at the mentioned color temperature. The abovementioned results are consistent with those reported by Yamagishi et al 41 who pointed to the effects of CCT conditions (8200, 5000, 5000, 2500 K: controlled lighting in 470lux) on young and old people using the NV (Night vision) test.

Studies in recent decades have assessed the effect of environmental factors on human comfort, health, and function. However, almost all of them considered the effect of only one factor, and there is a dearth of research on the combined effects of parameters in real conditions. Mohebbian et al. 20 have pointed out that in thermal comfort conditions (temperature 22°C), the increase of illuminance decreased reaction time and its error, indicating its positive effect on the reaction time. On the other hand, the results of the referred study demonstrated that increasing the temperature (37°C) and illuminance through increasing the reaction time and reaction time error of individuals can interfere with the cognitive process and reduce their performance.

Amiri et al 42 have indicated that although sound, heat, and light have no negative impact on physiological and cognitive function at their harmless and permissible levels, simultaneous exposure to their harmful levels in different combined conditions exerts a mutual impact on physiological and cognitive parameters (working memory and attention), acting independently with a separate mechanism or synergistically with a similar mechanism 42 . The interaction between light and heat, as well as their impact on cognitive functions, has also been shown in the study by Lucas et al 43 . Huang and Bavolar have also found that the combination of different environmental factors reduces performance. In other words, if their combined effects are more than those of their individual effect, they can intensify each other’s effects and have a similar effect mechanism; however, if the combined effects of environmental risk factors are equal to those of their individual effect, they will most likely have a separate effect 44,45 .

Hygge et al 46 have demonstrated that tasks were performed more rapidly but less accurately in the presence of the sound component. The stated study observed the significant association of sound, heat, and light with text reading and word recall. These interactions are indicating the theoretical possibility that sound, heat, and internal light directly affect cognitive processes without general or partial mediation, at least not in the way that the inverse U hypothesis suggests. In the same context, Monazzam et al 47 have observed that an increase in vibration acceleration significantly improved discomfort and heart rate; however, it did not affect the reaction time. The results of the referred study also suggested that vibration and illuminance did not have a significant combined effect on the variables of discomfort, heart rate, and reaction time.

Interpersonal sensitivity to light may affect consciousness, cognitive function, and sleep physiology differently 48 . The impact of light on a wide range of electrophysiological factors may vary from person to person. It has been specified that the function of the visual system is strongly influenced by gender differences 49,50 . Chellappa et al 51 have assessed the effect of gender differences on light perception, conscious attention, and sleep in humans. According to their results, in a task requiring sustained attention in blue-enriched white light compared to blue-free light, men had higher light perception and faster reaction time than women. The distribution of Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) reaction times (the number of RT observations between 500-100 ms) illustrated that light at 6500 K color temperature led to the movement toward a faster RT range than light at 2500 K color temperature for men.

Sunlight seems to be the most effective among various light sources since it contains a sufficient amount and a wide range of light. Natural light, due to its role in the production of vitamin D in human blood, can improve mental mood, attention, cognitive function, physical activity, sleep quality, and consciousness 52 . In their study, Shishegar et al 53 analyzed the effects of daylighting on the health and consciousness of workers and students. As illustrated by their results, the health, satisfaction, attention, and performance of workers and students are improved by natural light 52 .

Furthermore, Sahin et al 54 studies daylight exposure and its impacts on biomarkers, consciousness, and performance. They classified 13 subjects in illuminance ((low light (<5 lux), red light (max = 631 nm, 213 lux, 1.1W/m2), and white light (2568K, 361 lux, 1.1W /m2) conditions. The results of the mentioned study indicated that red light could increase the short-term performance, reduce reaction time significantly (P=0.05), and improve power in functional tests during a day. They also confirmed the hypothesis that exposure to daylight at long and narrow (red) or polychromatic wavelengths (2568 K) causes higher consciousness and shorter reaction time. The abovementioned results have been reported in similar studies performed by Figueiro et al. 55 and Lafrance et al 56 .

Reaction time is the very short time that elapses between the presentation of a stimulus and the recording of the subject response. In healthy individuals, it usually lasts from 10-12 cent seconds, appearing voluntary and reflectively 57 . In other words, reaction time is the elapsed time for a person to understand the situation and process a response 58 . Smolders et al. 11 have examined a mixed group of individuals (n=32) in different blocks through functional tests at two levels of illuminance (200lux or 1000lux at the eye level, 4000 K) for one hour in the morning and one hour in the afternoon. The results of the stated study showed that an increase in the illuminance (1000 lux, compared to 200lux) led to improved cognitive function, enhanced consciousness, less somnolence, more energy, and shorter reaction time.

Dehghan et al 59 indicated that after 90 min of exposure, simple, diagnostic, dichromatic selection, and two-tone selection reaction time were significant at all lighting levels (P<0.0001). According to the results of the referred study, after 90 min of exposure, the minimum and the maximum reaction time scores in illuminance were 200 lux and 1500 lux, respectively. In addition, the maximum and minimum response errors were at the level of 200 lux (0.5) and 500 lux (0.1), respectively. The removal response was also significant at different levels of illuminance (P=0.017); therefore, the maximum and minimum removal responses were reported at the level of 200 and 1500 lux (0.1), respectively. Chang (2013) has achieved similar results regarding the effects of light on attention and reaction time using the PVT test (psychomotor vigilance task) in people exposed to 1 lux illuminance, as compared to 90 lux 60 . Correa et al. have found that blue-enrich white light led to a greater improvement in reaction time with higher levels of baseline consciousness 61 .

"Attention" is a cognitive process defined as a selective focus on one aspect of the environment while ignoring others. It is also attributed to the allocation of resource processing 13 . The word "attention" can be defined in accordance with the number of errors made during a test; therefore, more assiduous attention during the test leads to fewer errors and vice versa 62 . An increase in attention function has been reported after 6.5 h of light exposure with short wavelengths (460 nm), compared to those with long wavelengths (550.55 nm) 63 . Dehghan et al 59 investigated the effects of different levels of illuminance on the rate of attention and reaction time in laboratory conditions. The findings of the stated study indicated that the maximum and minimum percentages of attention in 1500 and 500 lux illuminance equal 99.75% and 99.36%, respectively.

This finding is in line with that obtained by Smolders et al 11 who revealed that increasing illuminance improved cognitive functions in individuals. Amiri et al. 42 also pointed out that although the mean scores of working memory and attention in low light level exposure are lower than harmless level exposure, this difference is not statistically significant. A field study conducted in schools has demonstrated that classroom lighting with variable illuminance ("focused" program: very bright, cold light: 1060 lux) enhanced attention in students 64 . The majority of studies reported that higher illuminance (1000-5000 lux vs. 5-200 lux for 1-5 h) is associated with increased function and attention 65,66 . Kretschmer suggested that exposure to bright light at night reduced error rates in tasks requiring memory and focused performance; nonetheless, it was is completely ineffective in tasks requiring sustained attention 33 .


Conclusion

As evidenced by the results of the present study, it can be stated that lighting affects the attention and reaction time; therefore, it should be designed to meet non-visual needs, apart from comfort and visual requirements. The parameters of the wavelength, color temperature, and intensity of light modulate the brain responses, including attention and reaction time. The best light in the regulation of psychological, biological, and cognitive processes is bright daylight in the morning with a short wavelength, high intensity, as well as stronger and more lasting effects. Shorter wavelengths, compared to the longer ones, lead to suppressed Melatonin, higher consciousness, less somnolence, increased attention function, and faster reaction time. When exposed to monochromatic light, non-visual responses are most sensitive to blue light (wavelengths between 459 and 483 nm).

The effects of blue light on the enhancement of cognitive function have been reported in various studies; nonetheless, direct exposure to it may cause retinal damage and oxidative stress. Illuminance, in addition to wavelength, affects reaction time and attention. The majority of studies have reported higher illuminance to be associated with increased consciousness, decreased somnolence, increased attention, and faster reaction time. Moreover, the assessment of the effects of color temperature demonstrated that higher color temperature is associated with greater melatonin suppression and faster reaction time in tasks that require attention. Therefore, the design and use of light in workplaces should be performed to meet non-visual and cognitive needs, such as attention and reaction time, in addition to providing comfort and visual needs. The effects of light on attention and reaction time can be presented through the following conceptual model. ()

jrhs-21-e00529-g002
Figure 2. Conceptual model for the effects of light on attention and reaction time


Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Hamadan University of Medical Sciences for the library support of this study.


Conflict of interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.


Funding

Not funding.

Highlights

  • The light is a powerful modulator of non-visual performance in cognitive tasks.

  • The light with shorter wavelengths, higher intensity, and higher color temperature lead to increased attention and faster reaction time.

  • The best light in the regulation of psychological, biological, and cognitive processes is bright daylight in the morning with a short wavelength and high intensity.

  • Simultaneous exposure to harmful levels of environmental factors interacts with cognitive and physiological parameters.


References

  1. Lin JY, Sann SB, Zhou K, Nabavi S, Proulx CD, Malinow R. Optogenetic inhibition of synaptic release with chromophore-assisted light inactivation (CALI). Neuron 2013; 79:241-53.
  2. Ferlazzo F, Piccardi L, Burattini C, Barbalace M, Giannini A, Bisegna F. Effects of new light sources on task switching and mental rotation performance. J Environ Psychol 2014; 39:92-100.
  3. Kahya E. The effects of job characteristics and working conditions on job performance. Int J Ind Ergon 2007; 37:515-23.
  4. Küller R, Ballal S, Laike T, Mikellides B, Tonello G. The impact of light and colour on psychological mood: a cross-cultural study of indoor work environments. Ergonomics 2006; 49:1496-507.
  5. Lin CJ, Feng W-y, Chao C-j, Tseng F-Y. Effects of VDT workstation lighting conditions on operator visual workload. Ind Health 2008; 46:105-11.
  6. Heydarian A, Pantazis E, Carneiro JP, Gerber D, Becerik-Gerber B. Lights, building, action: Impact of default lighting settings on occupant behaviour. J Environ Psychol 2016; 48:212-23.
  7. Vickers NJ. Animal communication: when i’m calling you, will you answer too?. Curr Biol 2017; 27:13-5.
  8. Borisuit A, Linhart F, Scartezzini J-L, Münch M. Effects of realistic office daylighting and electric lighting conditions on visual comfort, alertness and mood. Lighting Research & Technology 2015; 47:192-209.
  9. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science 2002; 295:1070-3.
  10. Dijk D-J, Archer SN. Light, sleep, and circadian rhythms: together again. PLoS Biol 2009; 7:1-4.
  11. Smolders KC, De Kort YA, Cluitmans P. A higher illuminance induces alertness even during office hours: findings on subjective measures, task performance and heart rate measures. Physiol Behav 2012; 107:7-16.
  12. Vandewalle G, Maquet P, Dijk D-J. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13:429-38.
  13. Zarghi A, Zali A, Tehranidost M, Zarindast MR, Khodadadi SM. Application of cognitive computerized test in assessment of neuro-cognitive domain. Pajoohandeh Journal 2011; 16:241-5.
  14. Bonnett R. International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human Factors: Crc Press; 2001; 777-80.
  15. Yates PJ, Calder JD, Stranks GJ, Conn KS, Peppercorn D, Thomas NP. Early MRI diagnosis and non-surgical management of spontaneous osteonecrosis of the knee. Knee 2007; 14:112-6.
  16. Golbabaei F, Mazloumi A, Mamhood Khani S, Kazemi Z, Hosseini M, Abbasinia M. The effects of heat stress on selective attention and reaction time among workers of a hot industry: application of computerized version of stroop test. Health and safety at work 2015; 5:1-10.
  17. Adl J, Shokoohi Y, Kakooei H. Safety climate as an indicator to evaluate the performance of occupational health and safety management system. Journal of Health 2012; 3:32-40.
  18. Dehghan SF, Mehrifar Y, Sayedabadi E, Piran M, Aghl S, Motlagh AT. Investigating the Relationship between Exposure Level to Sound Pressure Level (SPL) and Light Intensity with Occupational Burnout in an Automotive Parts Industry. J Biochem 2020:18-24.
  19. Balazova I, Clausen G, Wyon DP, editors. The influence of exposure to multiple indoor environmental parameters on human perception, performance and motivation. Proceedings of Clima 2007 WellBeing Indoors. 2007.
  20. Mohebian Z, Mehrifar Y, Dehghan H, Habibi E, Yadegarfar G. Investigating the combined effects of heat and lighting on students reaction time in laboratory condition. Johe 2016; 3:40-6.
  21. Yang J, Zhang T, Lin Y, Xu W, editors. Effect of Illuminance and Light Strobe on Attention and Visual Fatigue in Indoor Lighting. 16th China International Forum on Solid State Lighting & 2019 International Forum on Wide Bandgap Semiconductors China; 25-27 Nov; Shenzhen, China 2019.
  22. Vandewalle G, Balteau E, Phillips C, Degueldre C, Moreau V, Sterpenich V. Daytime light exposure dynamically enhances brain responses. Curr Biol 2006; 16:1616-21.
  23. Leichtfried V, Mair-Raggautz M, Schaeffer V, Hammerer-Lercher A, Mair G, Bartenbach C. Intense illumination in the morning hours improved mood and alertness but not mental performance. Appl Ergon 2015; 46:54-9.
  24. Viola AU, James LM, Schlangen LJ, Dijk D-J. Blue-enriched white light in the workplace improves self-reported alertness, performance and sleep quality. Scand J Work Environ Health 2008:297-306.
  25. Phipps-Nelson J, Redman JR, Schlangen LJ, Rajaratnam SM. Blue light exposure reduces objective measures of sleepiness during prolonged nighttime performance testing. Chronobiol Int 2009; 26:891-912.
  26. Motamedzadeh M, Golmohammadi R, Kazemi R, Heidarimoghadam R. The effect of blue-enriched white light on cognitive performances and sleepiness of night-shift workers: a field study. Physiol Behav 2017; 177:208-14.
  27. Baek H, Min B-K. Blue light aids in coping with the post-lunch dip: an EEG study. Ergonomics 2015; 58:803-10.
  28. Studer P, Brucker JM, Haag C, Van Doren J, Moll GH, Heinrich H. Effects of blue-and red-enriched light on attention and sleep in typically developing adolescents. Physiol Behav 2019; 199:11-9.
  29. Knaier R, Schäfer J, Rossmeissl A, Klenk C, Hanssen H, Höchsmann C. Effects of bright and blue light on acoustic reaction time and maximum handgrip strength in male athletes: a randomized controlled trial. Eur J Appl Physiol 2017; 117:1689-96.
  30. Bansal N, Prakash NR, Randhawa JS, Kalra P. Effects of blue light on cognitive performance. Int Res J Eng Techmol 2017; 4:2434-42.
  31. Cajochen C, Chellappa SL, Schmidt C. Circadian and light effects on human sleepiness–alertness. Sleepiness and human impact assessment 2014; 9-22.
  32. Vetter C, Juda M, Lang D, Wojtysiak A, Roenneberg T. Blue-enriched office light competes with natural light as a zeitgeber. Scand J Work Environ Health 2011; 437-45.
  33. Kretschmer V, Schmidt K, Griefahn B. Bright light effects on working memory, sustained attention and concentration of elderly night shift workers. Lighting Research & Technology Journal 2012; 44:316-33.
  34. Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Blattner P, Oelhafen P, Götz T, Cajochen C. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert?. PloS One 2011; 6:1-11.
  35. Wu J, Seregard S, Algvere PV. Photochemical damage of the retina. Surv Ophthalmol 2006; 51:461-81.
  36. Sparrow JR, Cai B. Blue light–induced apoptosis of A2E-containing RPE: involvement of caspase-3 and protection by Bcl-2. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2001; 42:1356-62.
  37. Bozkurt F, Coskun H, Aydogan H. Effectiveness of Classroom Lighting Colors Toward Students’ Attention and Meditation Extracted From Brainwaves. Journal of Educational and Instructional Studies in the World 2014; 4:6-12.
  38. Deguchi T, Sato M. The effect of color temperature of lighting sources on mental activity level. Ann Physiol Anthropol 1992; 11:37-43.
  39. Hoffmann G, Gufler V, Griesmacher A, Bartenbach C, Canazei M, Staggl S. Effects of variable lighting intensities and colour temperatures on sulphatoxymelatonin and subjective mood in an experimental office workplace. Appl Ergon 2008; 39:719-28.
  40. Huang RH, Lee L, Chiu YA, Sun Y. Effects of correlated color temperature on focused and sustained attention under white LED desk lighting. Color Research & Application Journal 2015; 40:281-6.
  41. Yamagishi M, Yamaba K, Kubo C, Nokura K, Nagata M. Effects of LED lighting characteristics on visual performance of elderly people. Gerontechnology Journal 2008; 7:243-48.
  42. Amiri F, Zamanian Z, Mani A, Hasanzadeh J. Effects of combined exposure to noise, heat and lighting on cognitive performance. J Occup Health 2015; 12:10-20.
  43. Lucas RA, Epstein Y, Kjellstrom T. Excessive occupational heat exposure: a significant ergonomic challenge and health risk for current and future workers. Extreme physiology & medicine 2014; 3:1-8.
  44. Huang X-q, Li M, Yuan Y-y, Xiong Y-f, Zheng L. Experimental investigation on noise radiation characteristics of pulse detonation engine–driven ejector. Advances in Mechanical Engineering Journal 2015; 7:1-9.
  45. Bavolar J, Orosová Og. Decision-making styles and their associations with decision-making competencies and mental health. Judgment and Decision Making Journal 2015; 10:115-22.
  46. Hygge S, Knez I. Effects of noise, heat and indoor lighting on cognitive performance and self-reported affect. J Environ Psychol 2001; 21:291-9.
  47. Monazzam MR, Shoja E, Zakerian SA, Foroushani AR, Shoja M, Gharaee M. Combined effect of whole-body vibration and ambient lighting on human discomfort, heart rate, and reaction time. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2018; 91:537-45.
  48. Chellappa SL, Viola AU, Schmidt C, Bachmann V, Gabel V, Maire M. Human melatonin and alerting response to blue-enriched light depend on a polymorphism in the clock gene PER3. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2012; 97:33-7.
  49. Van Diepen HC, Foster RG, Meijer JH. A colourful clock. PLoS Biology 2015; 13:1-5.
  50. Hedera P, Wu D, Collins S, Lewin JS, Miller D, Lerner AJ. Sex and electroencephalographic synchronization after photic stimulation predict signal changes in the visual cortex on functional MR images. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1998; 19:853-7.
  51. Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Oelhafen P, Cajochen C. Sex differences in light sensitivity impact on brightness perception, vigilant attention and sleep in humans. Sci Rep 2017; 7:1-9.
  52. Shishegar N, Boubekri M, editors editors. Natural light and productivity: Analyzing the impacts of daylighting on students’ and workers’ health and alertness. International Journal of Advances in Chemical Engg, & Biological Sciences 2016; 3:72-7.
  53. Smolders KC, de Kort YA. Bright light and mental fatigue: Effects on alertness, vitality, performance and physiological arousal. J Environ Psychol 2014; 39:77-91.
  54. Sahin L, Wood BM, Plitnick B, Figueiro MG. Daytime light exposure: Effects on biomarkers, measures of alertness, and performance. Behav Brain Res 2014; 274:176-85.
  55. Figueiro M, Nonaka S, Rea M. Daylight exposure has a positive carryover effect on nighttime performance and subjective sleepiness. Lighting Research & Technology 2014; 46:506-19.
  56. Lafrance C, Dumont M, Lespérance P, Lambert C. Daytime vigilance after morning bright light exposure in volunteers subjected to sleep restriction. Physiol Behav 1998; 63:803-10.
  57. Kosinski RJ. A literature review on reaction time. Clemson University 2008; 1-10.
  58. Stranks JW. Human factors and behavioural safety: Routledge; 2007.
  59. Dehghan H, MOHEBIAN Z, Yadegarfar G. Evaluation of Effects of Different Levels of Brightness on Attention and Reaction Time under Laboratory Conditions. Journal of ergonomics 2017; 4:48-55.
  60. Chang A-M, Scheer FA, Czeisler CA, Aeschbach D. Direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans depend on prior light history. Sleep 2013; 36:1239-46.
  61. Correa Á, Barba A, Padilla F. Light effects on behavioural performance depend on the individual state of vigilance. PloS one 2016; 11:1-13.
  62. Marras WS. Fundamentals and assessment tools for occupational ergonomics: Crc Press; 2006.
  63. Lockley SW, Evans EE, Scheer FA, Brainard GC, Czeisler CA, Aeschbach D. Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 2006; 29:161-8.
  64. Barkmann C, Wessolowski N, Schulte-Markwort M. Applicability and efficacy of variable light in schools. Physiol Behav 2012; 105:621-7.
  65. Huiberts LM, Smolders KC, de Kort YA. Non-image forming effects of illuminance level: exploring parallel effects on physiological arousal and task performance. Physiol Behav 2016; 164:129-39.
  66. Phipps-Nelson J, Redman JR, Dijk D-J, Rajaratnam SM. Daytime exposure to bright light, as compared to dim light, decreases sleepiness and improves psychomotor vigilance performance. Sleep 2003; 26:695-700.
  67. Dehghan H. The relationship of sleep quality and mental fatigue in different levels of lighting on attention and reaction time in thermal comfort condition. Iran Occupational Health 2017; 14:85-94.
  68. Watson P, Korjoukov I, Vartak D, Roelfsema PR. Luminance contrast has little influence on the spread of object-based attention. Vision Res 2013; 85:90-103.
  69. Mohebian Z, Farhang Dehghan S, Dehghan H. Evaluation of the combined effects of heat and lighting on the level of attention and reaction time: climate chamber experiments in Iran. Scientific World Journal 2018; 1-8.
  70. Silva-Urra JA, Núñez-Espinosa CA, Niño-Mendez OA, Gaitán-Peñas H, Altavilla C, Toro-Salinas A. Circadian and Sex Differences After Acute High-Altitude Exposure: Are Early Acclimation Responses Improved by Blue Light?. Wilderness Environ Med 2015; 26:459-71.
  71. Askaripoor T, Motamedzadeh M, Golmohammadi R, Farhadian M, Babamiri M, Samavati M. Non-image forming effects of light on brainwaves, autonomic nervous activity, fatigue, and performance. J Circadian Rhythms 2018; 16:9-13.
  72. Sun C, Lian Z, Lan L. Work performance in relation to lighting environment in office buildings. Indoor Built Environ 2019; 28:1064-82.
  73. Golmohammadi R, Mehdinia M, Shahidi R, Darvishi E. The Effects of Lighting on Mental and Cognitive Performance: A Structured Systematic Review. Iranian Journal of Ergonomics 2017; 5:43-55.
Submitted: 26 Jan 2021
Revised: 12 Dec 2021
First published online: 31 Oct 2021
EndNote EndNote

(Enw Format - Win & Mac)

BibTeX BibTeX

(Bib Format - Win & Mac)

Bookends Bookends

(Ris Format - Mac only)

EasyBib EasyBib

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Medlars Medlars

(Txt Format - Win & Mac)

Mendeley Web Mendeley Web
Mendeley Mendeley

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Papers Papers

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

ProCite ProCite

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Reference Manager Reference Manager

(Ris Format - Win only)

Refworks Refworks

(Refworks Format - Win & Mac)

Zotero Zotero

(Ris Format - FireFox Plugin)

Abstract View: 614
PDF Download: 170
Full Text View: 147